The Science of Laughs
Laughing out loud: scientists have made
new discoveries about laughter.
Scanning brains and eavesdropping on
chimps, researchers are figuring out why we
chuckle, guffaw and crack up. Hint: it
isn’t funny.
By Sharon Begley, NEWSWEEK
NEWSWEEK October 9 issue — The man was
known around town as a chronic
borrower, always asking the neighbors to
lend him one tool or another. So when he
strolled over to Mr. Green one Sunday
and asked, “Say, are you using your lawn mower
today?” Green had a ready answer. “Yes,
I am,” he said emphatically.
TO WHICH THE would-be borrower replied, “Great!
Then since you won’t need your
golf clubs, I’ll just borrow those.”
If you are like most people, that punch
line triggered at least one short exhalation of
breath chopped into staccato segments
lasting about one fifteenth of a second each and
spaced one fifth of a second apart. In
other words, laughter. Exactly why humor causes
this acoustic response—rather than, say,
sweaty palms or any other physiological
reaction—has been lost in the mists of
evolution. Thinkers from Plato and Aristotle to
Kant, Darwin and Freud have tried to
fathom laughter (Plato feared it would disrupt the
state, and Kant theorized that it arises
when what you expect to happen doesn’t, which is
why surprising punch lines work), but
they’ve been long on philosophy and short on
science.
Lately, though, an intrepid band of
researchers has been trying to remedy that. With
techniques like MRI brain scans to probe
why people cannot tickle themselves into
paroxysms of laughter, they are tackling
“one of the last great unsolved problems in
human behavior,” says neuroscientist
Robert Provine of the University of Maryland,
whose book, “Laughter: A Scientific
Investigation,” will be published next week. And
no, he’s not referring to why guys are
always walking into bars with ducks under their
arms.
To investigate the roots of laughter,
scientists have turned to our primate cousins. If you
tickle a chimp (carefully; and
preferably a baby), it will likely laugh, but the sound
doesn’t resemble human laughter so much as
it does panting, with one sound per inhale
and exhale. Provine realized that the
reason chimps cannot emit a string of “ho ho ho’s”
is that they cannot make more than a
single sound when they exhale or inhale. Humans,
in contrast, can chop up a single
exhalation into multiple bursts of “ha ha ha”—or words.
(Speech results from chopping up an
exhalation into separate sounds.) “Laughter,” says
Provine, “is a probe into such
fundamental questions as why humans can speak but other
apes can’t.” No humanlike laughing, no
speaking.
The fact that chimps pant while playing
suggests that laughter evolved from the heavy
breathing that accompanies something
like playful wrestling. Ritualized panting—
laughter—then might have come to
represent the playful activity itself, signaling, “I’m
enjoying this.” For there is no question
that human laughter is a social behavior. (You
would have laughed more at the
borrow-the-mower joke if you had heard it read aloud
while in a group, rather than reading it
silently and alone.)
But what do titters communicate? Some
clues come from Provine’s collection of 1,200
“laugh episodes,” from eavesdropping in
public places. He finds that speakers laugh more
than listeners, and women laugh at men
more than vice versa. (Laughing behind their
backs doesn’t count.)
Laughter seems to signal an attempt to
ingratiate oneself: in India, notes Provine, men of
lower castes giggle when addressing men
of higher castes, but never the other way round.
People in power seldom giggle. More
evidence that laughing has less to do with humor
than with social signals is that, in
Provine’s 1,200 samples, by far the remarks that most
often elicited laughter were of the “it was nice
meeting you, too” or “I know” variety. In
other words, witless.
“Laughter is only rarely a response to
jokes,” says Provine. “It is, instead, the
quintessential human social signal. It
solidifies relationships and pulls people into the
fold.” And not only for the good. The
Columbine killers laughed as they shot,
demonstrating solidarity with each other
and, needless to say, not their targets. Evil
laughter is no oxymoron.
Laughter is contagious, for reasons that
remain pretty murky, but while you can catch it
from others you can’t induce it in
yourself. Forced laughter sounds, well, forced.
And tickling yourself is a non-starter.
There have been all sorts of theories about why, but
recent experiments led by Sarah-Jayne
Blakemore of University College, London, may
have finally revealed the answer. The
neuroscientists used functional MRI to
(noninvasively) peer into the brains of
volunteers who either tickled themselves or were
tickled by a robotic tickler. The MRI
detected more neuronal activity in the
somatosensory cortex, the part of the
brain that registers touch, when people were tickled
than when they tickled themselves. The
reason seems to be that when you move your
fingers to, say, tickle your rib cage,
the cerebellum, which coordinates complex
movements, predicts what that will feel
like—and sends out a signal to cancel the sensory
response much as the military can jam an
enemy radio transmission if it knows the
frequency ahead of time. But if the
tickle comes from someone else, and so is a surprise,
the cerebellum can’t block it.
Self-tickling thus seems to make neurons fire in a way that
blocks activation of the part of the
brain that ordinarily processes touch.
Laughter seems intimately entwined with
our physiology. It blocks a neural reflex that
regulates muscle tone, proving that “going
weak with laughter” is more than a metaphor.
Tumors or lesions of the brain’s
hypothalamus, which regulates basic processes like
respiration, can cause bouts of
uncontrolled laughter. And although laughter seems to
have evolved because of the message it
sends to others, it may have a therapeutic effect
on the laugher, too: it can raise heart
rate as much as aerobic exercise, lessen the
perception of pain and increase
tolerance of discomfort. While the scientists work out
how a good chuckle does all this, have
you heard the one about the priest, the minister
and the rabbi... ?
Aucun commentaire:
Enregistrer un commentaire